1: Attachment, Loss, and the Experience of Grief

Additional resources for this chapter

instructor material

DOI:

10.1891/9780826134752.0001

Abstract

This chapter deals with the attachment, loss, and the experience of grief. There is considerable writing in the psychological and psychiatric literature as to the nature of attachments—what they are and how they develop. One of the key figures and primary thinkers in this area is the late British psychiatrist John Bowlby. He devoted much of his professional career to the area of attachment and loss and wrote several substantial volumes as well as a number of articles on the subject. Bowlby’s attachment theory provides a way for us to conceptualize the tendency in human beings to create strong affectional bonds with others and a way to understand the strong emotional reaction that occurs when those bonds are threatened or broken. Normal grief, also referred to as uncomplicated grief, encompasses a broad range of feelings, cognitions, physical sensations, and behavioral changes that are common after a loss.

ATTACHMENT THEORY

Before one can fully comprehend the impact of a loss and the human behavior associated with it, one must have some understanding of the meaning of attachment. There is considerable writing in the psychological and psychiatric literature as to the nature of attachments—what they are and how they develop. One of the key figures and primary thinkers in this area is the late British psychiatrist John Bowlby. He devoted much of his professional career to the area of attachment and loss and wrote several substantial volumes as well as a number of articles on the subject.

Bowlby’s attachment theory provides a way for us to conceptualize the tendency in human beings to create strong affectional bonds with others and a way to understand the strong emotional reaction that occurs when those bonds are threatened or broken. To develop his theories, Bowlby casts his net wide and includes data from ethology, control theory, cognitive psychology, neurophysiology, and developmental biology. He takes exception to those who believe that attachment bonds between individuals develop only in order to have certain biological drives met, such as the drive for food or the drive for sex. Citing Lorenz’s work with animals and Harlow’s work with young monkeys, Bowlby (1977a) points to the fact that attachment occurs in the absence of the reinforcement of these biogenic needs.

Bowlby’s thesis is that these attachments come from a need for security and safety; they develop early in life, are usually directed toward a few specific individuals, and tend to endure throughout a large part of the life cycle. Forming attachments with significant others is considered normal behavior not only for the child but for the adult as well. Bowlby argues that attachment behavior has survival value, citing the occurrence of this behavior in the young of almost all species of mammals. But he sees attachment behavior as distinct from feeding and sexual behavior.

Attachment behavior is best illustrated by the young animal and the young child, who, as they grow, leave the primary attachment figure for increasingly long periods of time to search an ever-widening radius of their environment. But they always return to the attachment figure for support and safety. When the attachment figure disappears or is threatened, the response is one of intense anxiety and strong emotional protest. Bowlby suggests that the child’s parents provide the secure base of operation from which to explore. This relationship determines the child’s capacity to make affectional bonds later in life. This is similar to Erikson’s (1950) concept of basic trust; through good parenting, the individual sees himself as both able to help himself and worthy of being helped should difficulties arise. Obvious pathological aberrations can develop in this pattern. Inadequate parenting can lead people either to form anxious attachments or to form very tenuous attachments, if any at all (Winnicott, 1953, 1965). Various attachment styles can be found in Chapter 3.

If the goal of attachment behavior is to maintain an affectional bond, situations that endanger this bond give rise to certain very specific reactions. The greater the potential for loss, the more intense and the more varied these reactions are. “In such circumstances, all the most powerful forms of attachment behavior become activated—clinging, crying, and perhaps angry coercion.… When these actions are successful, the bond is restored; the activities cease and the states of stress and distress are alleviated” (Bowlby, 1977b, p. 429). If the danger is not removed, withdrawal, apathy, and despair then ensue.

Animals demonstrate this behavior as well as humans. In The Expression of Emotions in Man and Animals, written during the latter part of the 19th century, Darwin (1872) described the ways in which sorrow is expressed by animals as well as by children and adult human beings. Ethologist Lorenz (1963) has described this grief-like behavior in the separation of a greylag goose from its mate:

The first response to the disappearance of the partner consists in the anxious attempt to find him again. The goose moves about restlessly by day and night, flying great distances and visiting places where the partner might be found, uttering all the time the penetrating trisyllable long-distance call.… The searching expeditions are extended farther and farther and quite often the searcher itself gets lost, or succumbs to an accident. … All the objective observable characteristics of the goose’s behavior on losing its mate are roughly identical with human grief. (Lorenz, 1963, quoted in Parkes, 2001, p. 44)

There are many other examples of grieving in the animal world. Several years ago, there was an interesting account about dolphins in the Montreal zoo. After one of the dolphins died, its mate refused to eat, and the zookeepers had the difficult, if not impossible, task of keeping the surviving dolphin alive. By not eating, the dolphin was exhibiting manifestations of grief and depression akin to human loss behavior.

Psychiatrist George Engel, speaking at the psychiatric grand rounds at the Massachusetts General Hospital, described a case of bereavement in great detail. This case sounded typical of the kinds of reactions that you would find in a person who has lost a mate. Later in his lecture, after reading a lengthy newspaper account of this loss, Engel revealed that he was describing the behavior of an ostrich that had lost her mate!

Because of the many examples in the animal world, Bowlby concludes that there are good biological reasons for every separation to be responded to in an automatic, instinctive way with aggressive behavior. He also suggests that irretrievable loss is not taken into account, and that in the course of evolution, instinctual equipment developed around the fact that losses are retrievable and the behavioral responses that make up part of the grieving process are geared toward reestablishing a relationship with the lost object (Bowlby, 1980). This biological theory of grief has been influential in the thinking of many, including that of British psychiatrist Colin Murray Parkes (Parkes, 1972; Parkes & Stevenson-Hinde, 1982; Parkes & Weiss, 1983). Other prominent attachment theorists include Mary Ainsworth (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978) and Mary Main (Main & Hesse, 1990). The mourning responses of animals show what primitive biological processes are at work in humans. However, there are features of grieving specific only to human beings, and these normal grief reactions are described in this chapter (see Kosminsky & Jordan, 2016).

There is evidence that all humans grieve a loss to one degree or another. Anthropologists who have studied other societies, their cultures, and their reactions to the loss of loved ones report that whatever the society studied, in whatever part of the world, there is an almost universal attempt to regain the lost loved object and/or there is the belief in an afterlife where one can rejoin the loved one. In preliterate societies, however, bereavement pathology seems to be less common than it is in more civilized societies (Parkes, Laungani, & Young, 2015; Rosenblatt, 2008; Rosenblatt, Walsh, & Jackson, 1976).

IS GRIEF A DISEASE?

George Engel (1961) raised this interesting question in a thought-provoking essay published in Psychosomatic Medicine. Engel’s thesis is that the loss of a loved one is psychologically traumatic to the same extent that being severely wounded or burned is physiologically traumatic. He argues that grief represents a departure from the state of health and well-being, and just as healing is necessary in the physiological realm in order to bring the body back into homeostatic balance, a period of time is likewise needed to return the mourner to a similar state of psychological equilibrium. Therefore, Engel sees the process of mourning as similar to the process of healing. As with healing, full function, or nearly full function, can be restored, but there are also incidents of impaired function and inadequate healing. Just as the terms healthy and pathological apply to the various courses in the physiological healing process, Engel argues that these same terms may be applied to the courses taken by the mourning process. He sees mourning as a course that takes time until restoration of function can take place. How much functional impairment occurs is a matter of degree (Engel, 1961). Rather than using terms like restoration and recovery, I prefer to use the term adaptation: some people make a better adaptation to the loss while others make a less good adaptation. In Chapter 5, we look at complicated mourning, where individuals are making a less than adequate adaptation to the loss.

Before we look at the characteristics of normal grief, it would be useful to look at three terms that are often used interchangeably: grief, mourning, and bereavement. For purposes of common understanding, in this book I am using the term grief to indicate the experience of one who has lost a loved one to death. It is comprised of thoughts, feelings, behaviors, and physiological changes that vary in pattern and intensity over time. The term grief can be applied to other losses, but in this book, it primarily addresses losses due to death. Mourning is the term applied to the process that one goes through in adapting to the death of the person. The finality and consequences of the loss are understood and assimilated into the life of the mourner. Bereavement defines the loss to which the person is trying to adapt and the experience of having lost someone close.

NORMAL GRIEF

Normal grief, also referred to as uncomplicated grief, encompasses a broad range of feelings, cognitions, physical sensations, and behavioral changes that are common after a loss.1 One of the earliest attempts to look at normal grief reactions in any systematic way was done by Erich Lindemann (1944) when he was chief of psychiatry at the Massachusetts General Hospital.

In the Boston area, there are two Catholic colleges well known for their football rivalry. Back in the fall of 1942, they met for one of their traditional Saturday encounters. Holy Cross beat Boston College, and after the game many people went to Cocoanut Grove, a local nightclub, to celebrate. During the revelries, a busboy lit a match while trying to change a lightbulb and accidentally set a decorative palm tree on fire. Almost immediately, the whole nightclub, which was packed beyond its legal capacity, was engulfed in flames. Nearly 500 people lost their lives in that tragedy.

Afterward, Lindemann and his colleagues worked with the family members who had lost loved ones in that tragedy, and from these data and others he wrote his classic paper “Symptomatology and Management of Acute Grief” (1944). From his observations of 101 recently bereaved patients he discovered similar patterns, which he described as the pathognomonic characteristics of normal or acute grief:

  1. Somatic or bodily distress of some type

  2. Preoccupation with the image of the deceased

  3. Guilt relating to the deceased or circumstances of the death

  4. Hostile reactions

  5. The inability to function as one had before the loss

In addition to these five, he described a sixth characteristic exhibited by many patients: they appeared to develop traits of the deceased in their own behavior.

There are many limitations to Lindemann’s study. Some of these have been outlined by Parkes (2001), who points out that Lindemann does not present figures to show the relative frequency of the syndromes described. Lindemann also neglects to mention how many interviews he had with the patients, and how much time had passed between the interviews and the date of the loss. Nevertheless, this remains an important and much cited study.

What is of particular interest to me is that the bereaved we see today at the Massachusetts General Hospital exhibit behaviors very similar to those described by Lindemann more than 70 years ago. In a large number of people undergoing an acute grief reaction, we find some or most of the following phenomena. Because the list of normal grief behaviors is so extensive and varied, I have placed them under four general categories: feelings, physical sensations, cognitions, and behaviors. Anyone counseling the bereaved needs to be familiar with the broad range of behaviors that falls under the description of normal grief.

Feelings

Sadness

Sadness is the most common feeling found in the bereaved and really needs little comment. This feeling is not necessarily manifested by crying behavior, but often it is. Parkes and Weiss (1983) conjecture that crying is a signal that evokes a sympathetic and protective reaction from others and establishes a social situation in which the normal laws of competitive behavior are suspended. Some mourners have a fear of sadness, especially the fear of its intensity (Taylor & Rachman, 1991). It is not uncommon to hear a person say, “I lost it at the funeral.” Still others try to block sadness through excessive activity only to discover that the sadness comes out at night. Not allowing the sadness to be experienced, with or without tears, can frequently lead to complicated mourning (see Chapter 5).

Anger

Anger is frequently experienced after a loss. It can be one of the most confusing feelings for the survivor and as such is at the root of many problems in the grieving process (Cerney & Buskirk, 1991). A woman whose husband died of cancer said to me, “How can I be angry? He didn’t want to die.” The truth is that she was angry at him for dying and leaving her. If the anger is not adequately acknowledged, it can lead to complicated mourning.

This anger comes from two sources: from a sense of frustration that there was nothing one could do to prevent the death, and from a kind of regressive experience that occurs after the loss of someone close. You may have had this type of regressive experience when you were a very young child on a shopping trip with your mother. You were in a department store and suddenly you looked up to find that she had disappeared. You felt panic and anxiety until your mother returned, whereupon, rather than express a loving reaction, you hauled off and hit her. This behavior, which Bowlby sees as part of our genetic heritage, symbolizes the message “Don’t leave me again!”

In the loss of any important person there is a tendency to regress, to feel helpless, to feel unable to exist without the person, and then to experience the anger that goes along with these feelings of anxiety. The anger that the bereaved person experiences needs to be identified and appropriately targeted toward the deceased in order to make a healthy adaptation.

One of the riskiest maladaptations of anger is the posture of turning the anger inward against the self. In a severe case of retroflected anger, the person may be down on him- or herself and could develop severe depression or suicidal behavior. A more psychodynamic interpretation of this retroflected anger response was given by Melanie Klein (1940), who suggests that the triumph over the dead causes the bereaved person to turn his or her anger against him- or herself or direct it outward toward others nearby.

Blame

Anger is often handled in other less effective ways, one of which is displacement, or directing it toward some other person and often blaming him or her for the death (Drenovsky, 1994). The line of reasoning is that if someone can be blamed, then that person is responsible and, hence, the loss could have been prevented. People may blame the physician, the funeral director, family members, an insensitive friend, and frequently God. “I feel cheated but am confused not knowing who cheated me. God showed me something so precious and takes it away. Is this fair?” queried one widow (Exline, Park, Smyth, & Carey, 2011).

Field and Bonanno (2001) observed two types of blame in their research. One involved blaming the deceased, the second blaming themselves. Those who blamed the deceased experienced more anger and other symptoms in the early months after the death and had fewer continuing bonds. Those who blamed themselves experienced more grief symptoms of all kinds and had difficulty accepting the reality of the loss. They tended to keep the deceased’s possessions and to hold onto guilt that kept them connected to the deceased, rather than holding onto memories as a way of continuing attachments.

Guilt and Self-Reproach

Self-blame, shame, and guilt are common experiences of the bereaved and can affect grief outcomes (Duncan & Cacciatore, 2015). Guilt and self-reproach—over not being kind enough, over not taking the person to the hospital sooner, and the like—are frequently seen in survivors. Usually the guilt is manifested over something that happened or something that was neglected around the time of the death, something that may have prevented the loss (Li, Stroebe, Chan, & Chow, 2014). Most often the guilt is irrational and mitigates through reality testing. There is, of course, the possibility of real guilt, where the person has indeed done something to cause the death. In these cases, interventions other than reality testing would be called for.

Anxiety

Anxiety in the survivor can range from a light sense of insecurity to a strong panic attack, and the more intense and persistent the anxiety, the more it suggests an abnormal grief reaction (Onrust & Cuijpers, 2006). Anxiety comes primarily from two sources. The first source is attachment-related anxiety. This is the fear the survivor will not be able to take care of him- or herself and frequently comment “I won’t be able to survive without him (or her)” (Meier, Carr, Currier, & Neimeyer, 2013). Second, anxiety relates to a heightened sense of personal death awareness—the awareness of one’s own mortality increased by the death of a loved one (Worden, 1976). Carried to extremes, this anxiety can develop into a full-blown phobia. The well-known author C. S. Lewis (1961) knew this anxiety and said after losing his wife: “No one ever told me that grief felt so like fear. I am not afraid, but the sensation is like being afraid. The same fluttering in the stomach, the same restlessness, the yawning. I keep on swallowing” (p. 38).

Loneliness

Loneliness is a feeling frequently expressed by survivors, particularly those who have lost a spouse and who were used to a close day-to-day relationship. Even though very lonely, many widows will not go out because they feel safer in their homes. “I feel so all alone now,” said one widow who had been married for 52 years. “It’s been like the world has ended,” she told me 10 months after her husband’s death. W. Stroebe, Stroebe, Abakoumkin, and Schut (1996) distinguish between emotional loneliness and social loneliness. Social support can help with social loneliness but does not militate against emotional loneliness due to a broken attachment. The latter can only be remedied by the integration of another attachment (M. Stroebe, Schut, & Stroebe, 2005). Sometimes the need to be touched is a correlate of loneliness. This is especially true in cases of conjugal bereavement and often among the elderly (Van Baarsen, Van Duijn, Smit, Snijders, & Knipscheer, 2001).

Fatigue

Lindemann’s patients reported fatigue, and we see this frequently in survivors. It may sometimes be experienced as apathy or listlessness. This high level of fatigue can be both surprising and distressing to the person who is usually very active. “I can’t get out of bed in the morning,” said one widow. “I am neglecting the house because I am tired all the time.” Fatigue is usually self-limiting. If not, it may be a clinical sign of depression.

Helplessness

One factor that makes the event of death so stressful is the sense of helplessness it can engender. This close correlate of anxiety is frequently present in the early stage of a loss. Widows in particular often feel extremely helpless. A young widow left with a 7-week-old child said, “My family came and lived with me for the first 5 months. I was afraid I would freak out and not be able to care for my child.” Helplessness is associated with Locus of Control (internal versus external). Those with more external locus of control feel at the mercy of circumstances and less able to exert a sense of control and self-efficacy (Rubinstein, 2004).

Shock

Shock occurs most often in the case of a sudden death. Someone picks up the telephone and learns that a loved one or friend is dead. Even when the death is expected and follows a progressive deteriorating illness, when the phone call finally comes, it can still cause the survivor to experience shock and disbelief.

Yearning

Yearning for the lost person is what the British call pining. Parkes (2001; Parkes & Prigerson, 2010) has noted that pining is a common experience of survivors, particularly among the widows he studied. Yearning is a normal response to loss. When it diminishes, it may be a sign that mourning is coming to an end. When it does not come to an end, it may be a clinical sign indicating complicated mourning (W. Stroebe, Abakoumkin, & Stroebe, 2010). See Chapter 5 for a discussion of prolonged grief as one of the complications of mourning, and the place of yearning in its diagnosis (Robinaugh et al., 2016).

Emancipation

Emancipation can be a positive feeling after a death. I worked with a young woman whose father was a real potentate, a heavy-handed, unbending dictator over her existence. After his sudden death from a heart attack, she went through the normal grief feelings, but she also expressed a feeling of emancipation because she no longer had to live under his tyranny. At first, she was uncomfortable with this feeling, but later she was able to accept it as the normal response to her changed status.

Relief

Many people feel relief after the death of a loved one, particularly if the loved one suffered a lengthy or particularly painful illness. “The knowing that his suffering, both physical and mental, is over helps me cope,” said one elderly widow. This can also occur when the death involves a person with whom the mourner has had a particularly difficult and often lifelong relationship. Sometimes relief is the reaction following a completed suicide after a long series of suicide attempts. However, a sense of guilt often accompanies this sense of relief.

Numbness

It’s also important to mention that some people report a lack of feelings. After a loss, they feel numb. Again, this numbness is often experienced early in the grieving process, usually right after the person learns of the death. It probably occurs because there are so many feelings to deal with, that to allow them all into consciousness would be overwhelming, so the person experiences numbness as a protection from this flood of feelings. Commenting on numbness, Parkes and Weiss (1983) have said, “We found no evidence that it is an unhealthy reaction. Blocking of sensation as a defense against what would otherwise be overwhelming pain would seem to be extremely ‘normal’” (p. 55).

As you review this list, remember that each of these items listed represents normal grief feelings and there is nothing pathological about any one of them. However, feelings that exist for abnormally long periods of time and at excessive intensity may portend a complicated grief reaction. This is discussed in Chapter 5.

Physical Sensations

One of the interesting things about Lindemann’s paper is that he describes not only the emotions that people experienced, but also the physical sensations associated with their acute grief reactions. These sensations are often overlooked, but they play a significant role in the grieving process. The following is a list of the most commonly reported sensations experienced by the people we see for grief counseling:

  1. Hollowness in the stomach

  2. Tightness in the chest

  3. Tightness in the throat

  4. Oversensitivity to noise

  5. A sense of depersonalization: “I walk down the street and nothing seems real, including me.”

  6. Breathlessness, feeling short of breath

  7. Weakness in the muscles

  8. Lack of energy

  9. Dry mouth

Many times, these physical sensations are of concern to the survivor, and he or she may come to the physician for a checkup. If so, physicians need to inquire about deaths and losses as part of their diagnostic evaluation.

Cognitions

There are many different thought patterns that mark the experience of grief. Certain thoughts are common in the early stages of grieving and usually disappear after a short time. But sometimes thoughts persist and trigger feelings that can lead to depression or anxiety.

Disbelief

“It didn’t happen. There must be some mistake. I can’t believe it happened. I don’t want to believe it happened.” These are often the first thoughts to occur after hearing of a death, especially if the death was sudden. One young widow said to me, “I keep waiting for someone to wake me and tell me I’m dreaming.” Another said, “The passing of my husband came as a shock although he had been sick for some time. You are never quite ready for it.”

Confusion

Many newly bereaved people say their thinking is very confused, they can’t seem to order their thoughts, they have difficulty concentrating, or they forget things. I once went out for a social evening in Boston and took a cab home. I told the driver where I wanted to go and sat back while he proceeded down the road. A little later he asked me again where I wanted to go. I thought maybe he was a new driver and did not know the city, but he commented to me that he had a lot on his mind. A little later he asked again and then apologized and said that he was feeling very confused. This happened several more times, and finally I decided it would not hurt to ask him what was on his mind. He told me that his son had been killed the week before in a traffic accident.

Preoccupation

Preoccupations can be obsessive thoughts about the deceased. These often include obsessive thoughts about how to recover the lost person. Sometimes preoccupation takes the form of intrusive thoughts or images of the deceased suffering or dying. In our Harvard Child Bereavement Study, surviving parents with the highest levels of intrusive thoughts were those who unexpectedly lost a spouse with whom they had a highly conflicted relationship (Worden, 1996). Rumination is another form of preoccupation. People engaging in ruminative coping think persistently and repetitively about how bad they feel and about the circumstances that precipitated their feelings (Eisma et al., 2015; Nolen-Hoeksema, 2001).

Sense of Presence

This is the cognitive counterpart to the experience of yearning. The grieving person may think that the deceased is somehow still in the current area of time and space. This can be especially true during the time shortly after the death. In our study of bereaved children, 81% of the children felt watched by their dead parent 4 months after the death, and this experience continued for many of the children (66%) 2 years after the death. Some found this sense of presence comforting, while others did not and were scared by it (Worden, 1996).

Hallucinations

Hallucinations of both the visual type and the auditory type are included in this list of normal behaviors because hallucinations can be a frequent experience of the bereaved. They are usually transient illusory experiences, often occurring within a few weeks following the loss, and generally do not portend a more difficult or complicated mourning experience. Although disconcerting to some, many others find these experiences comforting. With all the interest in mysticism and spirituality, it is interesting to speculate whether these are really hallucinations or possibly some other kind of metaphysical phenomenon (Kersting, 2004).

There is an obvious interface between thinking and feeling, and the current interest in cognitive psychology and cognitive therapy emphasizes this. Aaron Beck and his colleagues (1979) at the University of Pennsylvania found that the experience of depression frequently is triggered by depressive thought patterns. In the bereaved, certain thoughts will pass through the mind such as “I can’t live without her” or “I’ll never find love again.” These thoughts can then trigger very intense, but normal, feelings of sadness and/or anxiety.

Behaviors

There are a number of specific behaviors frequently associated with normal grief reactions. These can range from sleep and appetite disturbances to absentmindedness and social withdrawal. The following behaviors are commonly reported after a loss and usually correct themselves over time.

Sleep Disturbances

It is not unusual for people who are in the early stages of loss to experience sleep disturbances. These may include difficulty going to sleep or early morning awakening. Sleep disturbances sometimes require medical intervention, but in normal grief they usually correct themselves. In the Harvard Child Bereavement Study, one-fifth of the children showed some sleep disturbance in the first 4 months after the death of one of their parents. Without any special intervention, this figure dropped to a level not significantly different from that of their nonbereaved matched counterparts 1 and 2 years following the death (Worden, 1996).

After Bill lost his wife suddenly, he would wake up at five o’clock each morning filled with intense sadness and review over and over the circumstances surrounding the death and how it might have been prevented, including what he might have done differently. This happened morning after morning and soon caused problems because he could not function well at work. After about 6 weeks, the disorder began to correct itself, and eventually it disappeared. This is not an unusual experience. However, if sleep disorder persists, it may indicate a more serious depressive disorder, which should be explored (Tanimukai et al., 2015). Sleep disorders can sometimes symbolize various fears, including the fear of dreaming, the fear of being in bed alone, and the fear of not awakening. After her husband died, one woman solved the problem posed by her fear of being alone in bed by taking her dog to bed with her. The sound of the dog’s breathing comforted her, and she continued to do this for almost a year until she was able to sleep alone.

Eating Disturbances

Bereaved animals exhibit eating disturbances, which are also very common in human mourning situations. Although appetite disturbances can manifest themselves in terms of both overeating and undereating, undereating is the more frequently described grief behavior. Significant changes in weight may result from changes in eating patterns.

Distracted and Absentminded Behavior

The newly bereaved may find themselves acting in an absentminded way or doing things that may ultimately cause them inconvenience or harm. One client was concerned because on three separate occasions she had driven across the city in her car and, after completing her business, had forgotten that she had driven and returned home via public transportation. This behavior occurred following the death of a close friend and eventually corrected itself.

Social Withdrawal

It is not unusual for people who have sustained a loss to want to withdraw from other people. Again, this is usually a short-lived phenomenon and corrects itself. I saw one young woman shortly after the death of her mother. This single woman was a very sociable person who loved to go to parties. For several months following her mother’s death, she declined all invitations because they seemed dissonant to the way she felt in the early experiences of her grief. This may seem obvious and appropriate to the reader, but this woman saw her withdrawal as abnormal. Some people withdraw from friends perceived as oversolicitous. “My friends tried so hard that I wanted to avoid them. How many times can you hear, ‘I’m sorry?’” Social withdrawal can also include a loss of interest in the outside world, such as not reading newspapers or watching television. Bereaved children who have lost a parent to death can also experience social withdrawal in the early months after the death (Silverman & Worden, 1993).

Dreams of the Deceased

It’s very common to dream of the dead person, both normal kinds of dreams and distressing dreams or nightmares. Often these dreams serve a number of purposes and may give some diagnostic clues as to where the person is in the whole course of mourning (Cookson, 1990).

For example, for several years after the death of her mother, Esther suffered from intense guilt over circumstances related to the death. This guilt was manifested in low self-esteem and personal recrimination and was associated with considerable anxiety. During one of her daily visits to her mother in the hospital, Esther had left the bedside for coffee and a bite of food. While she was out, her mother died.

Esther was filled with remorse, and although we used the usual reality-testing techniques in therapy, the guilt still persisted. While in therapy, she had a dream about her mother. In this dream, she saw herself trying to assist her mother to walk down a slippery pathway so she would not fall. But her mother fell, and nothing Esther could do in the dream would save her. It was impossible. This dream was a significant turning point in her therapy because she allowed herself to see that nothing she could have done would have kept her mother from dying. This important insight gave her permission to shed the guilt that she had been carrying for several years. Some ways to utilize dreams in grief counseling and grief therapy are presented in Chapter 6.

Avoiding Reminders of the Deceased

Some people will avoid places or things that trigger painful feelings of grief. They might avoid the place where the deceased died, the cemetery, or objects that remind them of their lost loved one. One middle-aged woman came for grief counseling when her husband died after a series of coronary attacks, leaving her with two children. For a period of time she put all pictures of her husband away in the closet, along with other things that reminded her of him. This obviously was only a short-term solution, and as she moved toward a better adaptation of her grief, she was able to bring out the items that she wanted to live with and display his picture on the piano.

Quickly getting rid of all the things associated with the deceased—giving them away or disposing of them in any way possible even to the point of having a quick disposal of the body—can lead to a complicated grief reaction. This is usually not healthy behavior and is often indicative of a highly ambivalent relationship with the deceased. Ambivalent relationships are one of the mediators of mourning described in Chapter 3.

Searching and Calling Out

Both Bowlby and Parkes have written much in their work about searching behavior. Calling out is related to this searching behavior. Frequently somebody may call out the name of the loved person: “John, John, John. Please come back to me!” When this is not done verbally, it can be going on subvocally.

Sighing

Sighing is a behavior frequently noted among the bereaved. It is a close correlate of the physical sensation of breathlessness. Colleagues at the Massachusetts General Hospital tested respiration in a small group of bereaved parents and found that their oxygen and carbon dioxide levels were similar to those found in depressed patients (Jellinek, Goldenheim, & Jenike, 1985).

Restless Hyperactivity

A number of widows in our Harvard studies of bereavement entered into restless hyperactivity following the deaths of their husbands. The woman mentioned previously whose husband left her with two teenage children could not stand to stay at home. She would get into her car and drive all over town trying to find some sense of relief from her restlessness. Another widow could stay in the house during the day because she was busy, but at night she fled.

Crying

There has been interesting speculation that tears may have potential healing value. Stress causes chemical imbalances in the body, and some researchers believe that tears remove toxic substances and help reestablish homeostasis. They hypothesize that the chemical content of tears caused by emotional stress is different from that of tears secreted as a function of eye irritation. Tests are being done to see what type of catecholamine (mood-altering chemicals produced by the brain) is present in tears of emotion (Frey, 1980). Tears do relieve emotional stress, but how they do this is still a question. Further research is needed on the deleterious effects, if any, of suppressed crying. Martin (2012), who has worked with individuals and families experiencing grief from traumatic events, has written an interesting paper titled “Grief That Has No Vent in Tears Makes Other Organs Weep.” This helps us to understand how highly traumatic experiences, emotionally and cognitively unprocessed, may become bodily expressed.

Visiting Places or Carrying Objects That Remind the Survivor of the Deceased

This is the opposite of the behavior that people engage in to avoid reminders of the lost person. Often underlying this behavior is the fear of losing memories of the deceased. “For 2 weeks I carried his picture with me constantly for fear I would forget his face,” one widow told me.

Treasuring Objects That Belonged to the Deceased

One young woman went through her mother’s closet shortly after her mother died and took many of her clothes home. They wore the same size, and although this might seem like an example of someone being thrifty, the fact was that the daughter did not feel comfortable unless she was wearing something that had belonged to her mother. She wore these clothes for several months. As her mourning progressed, she found it less and less necessary to wear clothing that had belonged to her mother. Finally, she gave most of it away to charity.

The reason for outlining these characteristics of normal grief in such detail is to show the wide variety of behaviors and experiences associated with loss. Obviously, not all these behaviors will be experienced by one person. However, it is important for bereavement counselors to understand the wide range of behaviors covered under normal grief, so they do not pathologize behavior that should be recognized as normal. Having this understanding will also enable counselors to give reassurance to people who experience such behavior as disturbing, especially in the case of their first significant loss. However, if these experiences persist late in the bereavement process, they may be indicative of a more complicated grief (Demi & Miles, 1987).

GRIEF AND DEPRESSION

Many of the normal grief behaviors may seem like manifestations of depression. To shed some light on this, let’s look at the debate about the similarities and differences between grief and depression.

Freud (1917/1957), in his early paper “Mourning and Melancholia,” addressed this issue. He tried to point out that depression, or melancholia, as he called it, is a pathological form of grief and is very much like mourning (normal grief) except that it has a certain characteristic feature of its own—namely, angry impulses toward the ambivalently loved person turned inward. It is true that grief looks very much like depression, and it is also true that grieving may develop into a full-blown depression. Klerman (Kierman & Izen, 1977; Klerman & Weissman, 1986), who was a prominent depression researcher, believed that many depressions are precipitated by losses, either immediately following the loss or at some later time when the patient is reminded of the loss. Depression may also serve as a defense against mourning. If anger is directed against the self, it is deflected away from the deceased, and this keeps the survivor from dealing with ambivalent feelings toward the deceased (Dorpat, 1973).

The main distinctions between grief and depression are these: In depression as well as grief, you may find the classic symptoms of sleep disturbance, appetite disturbance, and intense sadness; however, in a grief reaction, there is not the loss of self-esteem commonly found in most clinical depressions. That is, the people who have lost someone do not have less regard for themselves as a result of such a loss, or if they do, it tends to be for only a brief time. And if the survivors of the deceased experience guilt, it is usually guilt associated with some specific aspect of the loss rather than a general overall sense of culpability. Even though grief and depression share similar objective and subjective features, they do seem to be different conditions. Depression overlaps with bereavement but is not the same (Robinson & Fleming, 1989, 1992; Wakefield & Schmitz, 2013; Worden & Silverman, 1993; Zisook & Kendler, 2007). Freud believed that in grief, the world looks poor and empty, while in depression, the person feels poor and empty. These differences in cognitive style have been identified by Beck and associates (1979) and other cognitive therapists who have suggested that the depressed have negative evaluations of themselves, the world, and the future. Although such negative evaluations can exist in the bereaved, they tend to be more transient.

However, there are some bereaved individuals who do develop major depressive episodes (MDE) following a loss (Zisook, & Kendler, 2007; Zisook, Paulus, Shucter & Judd, 1997; Zisook & Shuchter, 1993, 2001). The recent Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (5th ed.; DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013) allows for this distinction. Zisook and colleagues (2012), who were influential in the dropping of the previous 2-month bereavement exclusion where depression could not be diagnosed in the bereaved until 2 months after the death, argue that, “the preponderance of data suggests that bereavement-related depression is not different from MDE that presents in any other context; it is equally genetically influenced, most likely to occur in individuals with past personal and family histories of MDE, has similar personality characteristics and patterns of comorbidity, is as likely to be chronic and/or recurrent, and responds to antidepressant medications.” If a major depressive episode develops during bereavement, this should be considered a type of complicated mourning—exaggerated grief (see Chapter 5).

At Yale, Jacobs, Hansen, Berkman, Kasi, and Ostfeld (1989), Jacobs et al. (1990), and Jacobs, Nelson, and Zisook (1987) have been interested in depression within the context of bereavement. According to them, “Although the majority of depressions of bereavement are transient and require no professional attention, there is growing appreciation that some depressions, especially those that persist throughout the first year of bereavement, are clinically significant” (1987, p. 501). They have used antidepressant medication to treat patients whose depression persisted late into the course of bereavement and did not resolve spontaneously or respond to interpersonal interventions. These were usually people who had a history of depression or some other mental health disorder. They found improvement in sleep disorders and appetite disturbance as well as an improvement in mood and cognition. This response suggests a biological dimension to the depression.

A section in the DSM-5 (APA, 2013) advises that:

Responses to a significant loss [such as bereavement] may include the feeling of intense sadness, rumination about the loss, insomnia, poor appetite and weight loss which may resemble a depressive episode. Although such symptoms may be understandable or considered appropriate to the loss, the presence of a major depressive episode in addition to the normal response to a significant loss should also be carefully considered. This decision inevitably requires the exercise of clinical judgement based on the individual’s history and the cultural norms for the expression of distress in the context of loss. (p. 95)

One of the functions of the counselor who has contact with people during the time of acute grief is to assess which patients might be undergoing a major depression by using current standard diagnostic criteria from the DSM-5 (APA, 2013). Patients so identified can then be given additional help such as a medical evaluation and possibly the use of antidepressant medications. Once depression begins to lift through medication, then the focus of treatment changes to the underlying conflicts of the attachment. These conflicts cannot be addressed through medications alone (Miller et al., 1994).

If grief is defined as one’s experiences after a loss, then mourning is the process one goes through leading to an adaptation to the loss. In the next two chapters, we look at the mourning process in detail.

FOR REFLECTION AND DISCUSSION

  • In this chapter the terms grief, bereavement, and mourning are defined. How does this distinction make this topic more understandable for you? In what ways would you modify these definitions?

  • Looking at the diversity of (a) feelings, (b) physical sensations, (c) cognitions, and (d) behaviors that are typical of normal, uncomplicated grief, which of these have you witnessed most frequently in your work with bereaved individuals? Which have you experienced yourself in the aftermath of a significant loss?

  • Bereaved individuals sometimes report a sense they are losing their mind. How do the cognitions and emotions described in this chapter contribute to this sense of craziness?

  • Behaviors such as carrying around items that belonged to the deceased might cause some well-meaning family members and friends to think the bereaved person needs professional help. How could you reassure your clients about the normalcy of these behaviors?

  • What might be some of the problems when treating normal bereavement reactions as if they were the symptomatic criteria for major depressive disorder? How might these problems be significant in clinical practice and why?

1 I am using the word normal in both a clinical and a statistical sense. Clinical defines what the clinician calls normal mourning behavior, while statistical refers to the frequency with which such behavior is found among a randomized bereaved population. The more frequent the behavior, the more it is defined as normal.

REFERENCES

  1. Ainsworth, M., Blehar, M., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of strange situations. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  2. American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
  3. Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy of depression. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
  4. Bowlby, J. (1977a). The making and breaking of affectional bonds: I. Aetiology and psychopathology in the light of attachment theory. British Journal of Psychiatry, 130, 201210. doi:10.1192/bjp.130.3.201
  5. Bowlby, J. (1977b). The making and breaking of affectional bonds: II. Some principles of psychotherapy: The fiftieth Maudsley lecture (expanded edition). British Journal of Psychiatry, 130, 421431. doi:doi:10.1192/bjp.130.5.421
  6. Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss: Loss, sadness, and depression (Vol. II). New York, NY: Basic Books.
  7. Cerney, M. S., & Buskirk, J. R. (1991). Anger: The hidden part of grief. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 55, 228237.
  8. Cookson, K. (1990). Dreams and death: An exploration of the literature. OMEGA–Journal of Death and Dying, 21, 259281. doi:10.2190/8LJ8-6GN1-YT22-B8D4
  9. Darwin, C. (1872). The expression of emotions in man and animals. London, UK: Murray.
  10. Demi, A. S., & Miles, M. S. (1987). Parameters of normal grief: A Delphi study. Death Studies, 11, 397412. doi:10.1080/07481188708252206
  11. Dorpat, T. L. (1973). Suicide, loss, and mourning. Life-Threatening Behavior, 3, 213224. doi:10.1111/j.1943-278X.1973.tb00867.x
  12. Drenovsky, C. K. (1994). Anger and the desire for retribution among bereaved parents. OMEGA–Journal of Death and Dying, 29, 303312. doi:10.2190/HT0E-HCHE-JFLP-XG7F
  13. Duncan, C., & Cacciatore, J. (2015). A systematic review of the peer-reviewed literature on self-blame, guilt, and shame. OMEGA–Journal of Death and Dying, 71, 312342. doi:10.1177/0030222815572604
  14. Eisma, M., Schut, H., Stroebe, M., Boelen, P., van den Bout, J., & Stroebe, W. (2015). Adaptive and maladaptive rumination after loss: A three-wave longitudinal study. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 54, 163180. doi:10.1111/bjc.12067
  15. Engel, G. L. (1961). Is grief a disease? A challenge for medical research. Psychosomatic Medicine, 23, 1822.
  16. Erikson, E. H. (1950). Childhood and society. New York, NY: W. W. Norton.
  17. Exline, J. J., Park, C. L., Smyth, J. M., & Carey, M. P. (2011). Anger toward God: Social-cognitive predictors, prevalence, and links with adjustment to bereavement and cancer. Journal of Personality and Social Psvcholoqy, 100, 129148. doi:10.1037/a0021716
  18. Field, N., & Bonanno, G. (2001). The role of blame in adaptation in the first five years following the death of a spouse. American Behavioral Scientist, 44, 764781.
  19. Freud, S. (1957). Mourning and melancholia. In J. Strachey (Ed. and Trans.), Standard edition of the complete works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 14, pp. 237260). New York, NY: Basic Books. (Original work published 1917)
  20. Frey, W. H. (1980). Not-so-idle-tears. Psychology Today, 13, 9192.
  21. Jacobs, S., Hansen, F., Berkman, L., Kasi, S., & Ostfeld, A. (1989). Depressions of bereavement. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 30, 218224. doi:10.1016/0010-440X(89)90041-2
  22. Jacobs, S., Hansen, F., Kasl, S., Ostfeld, A., Berkman, L., & Kim, K. (1990). Anxiety disorders during acute bereavement: Risk and risk factors. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 51, 269274.
  23. Jacobs, S., Nelson, J., & Zisook, S. (1987). Treating depression of bereavement with antidepressants: A pilot study. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 10, 501510.
  24. Jellinek, M. S., Goldenheim, P., & Jenike, M. (1985). The impact of grief on ventilatory control. American Journal of Psychiatry, 142, 121123. doi:10.1176/ajp.142.1.121
  25. Kersting, A. (2004). The psychodynamics of grief hallucinations—a psychological phenomenon of normal and pathological grief. Psychopathology, 37, 5051. doi:10.1159/000077020
  26. Klein, M. (1940). Mourning and its relationship to manic-depressive states. International Journal Psychoanalysis, 21, 125153. Retrieved from https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/1216/dd85933628fac2775a408346a790c43fd45e.pdf
  27. Klerman, G. L., & Izen, J. (1977). The effects of bereavement and grief on physical health and general well being. Advances in Psychosomatic Medicine, 9, 63104.
  28. Klerman, G., & Weissman, M. (1986). The interpersonal approach to understanding depression. In T. Million & G. Klerman (Eds.), Contemporary directions in psychopathology: Toward the DSM-IV (pp. 429456). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
  29. Kosminsky, P., & Jordan, J. (Eds.). (2016). Attachment-informed grief therapy: The clinician’s guide to foundations and applications. New York, NY: Routledge.
  30. Lewis, C. S. (1961). A grief observed. London, UK: Faber & Faber.
  31. Li, J., Stroebe, M., Chan, C., & Chow, A. (2014). Guilt in bereavement: A review and conceptual framework. Death Studies, 38, 165171. doi:10.1080/07481 187.2012.738770
  32. Lindemann, E. (1944). Symptomatology and management of acute grief. American Journal of Psychiatry, 101, 141148. doi:10.1176/ajp.101.2.141
  33. Lorenz, K. (1963). On aggression. London, UK: Methuen.
  34. Main, M., & Hesse, E. (1990). Parents’ unresolved traumatic experiences are related to infant disorganized attachment status: Is frightened and/or frightening parental behavior the linking mechanism? In M. T. Greenberq, D. Cicchetti, & M. E. Cumminqs (Eds.), Attachment in the preschool years: Theory, research, and intervention (pp. 161182). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
  35. Martin, P. (2012). “Grief that has no vent in tears, makes other organs weep.” Seeking refuge from trauma in the medical setting. Journal of Child Psychotherapy, 38, 321. doi:10.1080/0075417X.2011.651839
  36. Meier, A., Carr, D., Currier, J., & Neimeyer, R. (2013). Attachment anxiety and avoidance in coping with bereavement: Two studies. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 32, 315334. Ddoi:10.1521/jscp.2013.32.3.315
  37. Miller, M. D., Frank, E., Cornes, C., Imber, S. D., Anderson, B., Ehrenpreis, L., … Reynolds, C. F., III (1994). Applying interpersonal psychotherapy to bereavement-related depression following loss of a spouse in late life. Journal of Psychotherapy Practice and Research, 3, 149162.
  38. Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2001). Ruminative coping and adjustment to bereavement. In M. S. Stroebe, R. O. Hansson, W. Stroebe, & H. Schut (Eds.), Handbook of bereavement research: Consequences, coping, and care (pp. 545562). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
  39. Onrust, S. A., & Cuijpers, P. (2006). Mood and anxiety disorders in widowhood: A systematic review. Aging & Mental Health, 10, 327334. doi:10.1080/13607860600638529
  40. Parkes, C. M. (1972). Bereavement: Studies of grief in adult life. New York, NY: International Universities Press.
  41. Parkes, C. M. (2001). A historical overview of the scientific study of bereavement. In M. S. Stroebe, R. O. Hansson, W. Stroebe, & H. Schut (Eds.), Handbook of bereavement research: Consequences, coping, and care (pp. 2545). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
  42. Parkes, C. M., Laungani, P., & Young, B. (2015). Death and bereavement across cultures (2nd ed.). London, UK: Routledge.
  43. Parkes, C. M. & Prigerson, H. (Eds.). (2010). Bereavement: Studies of grief in adult life (4th ed.). New York, NY: Routledge.
  44. Parkes, C. M., & Stevenson-Hinde, J. (Eds.). (1982). The place of attachment in human behavior. New York, NY: Basic Books.
  45. Parkes, C. M., & Weiss, R. S. (1983). Recovery from bereavement. New York, NY: Basic Books.
  46. Robinaugh, D., Mauro, C., Bui, E., Stone, L., Shah, R., Wang, Y., & Simon, N. (2016). Yearning and its measurement in complicated grief. Journal of Loss and Trauma, 21, 410420. doi:10.1080/15325024.2015.1110447
  47. Robinson, P. J., & Fleming, S. (1989). Differentiating grief and depression. Hospice Journal, 5, 7788. doi:10.1080/0742-969X.1989.11882640
  48. Robinson, P. J., & Fleming, S. (1992). Depressotypic cognitive patterns in major depression and conjugal bereavement. OMEGA–Journal of Death and Dying, 25, 291305. doi:10.2190/5EX9-086T-VVD4-EV9V
  49. Rosenblatt, P. (2008). Grief across cultures. In M. Stroebe, R. Hansson, H. Schut, & W. Stroebe (Eds.), Handbook of bereavement research and practice: Advances in theory and intervention (pp. 207221). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
  50. Rosenblatt, P. C., Walsh, R. P., & Jackson, D. A. (1976). Grief and mourning in cross-cultural perspective. New Haven, CT: Human Relations Area Files Press.
  51. Rubinstein, G. (2004). Locus of control and helplessness: Gender differences among bereaved parents. Death Studies, 28, 211223. doi:10.1080/07481180490276553
  52. Silverman, P., & Worden, J. W. (1993). Children’s reactions to the death of a parent. In M. S. Stroebe, W. Stroebe, & R. O. Hansson (Eds.), Handbook of bereavement: Theory, research, and intervention (pp. 300316). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
  53. Stroebe, M., Schut, H., & Stroebe, W. (2005). Attachment in coping with bereavement: A theoretical integration. Review of General Psychology, 9, 4866. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.9.1.48
  54. Stroebe, W., Abakoumkin, G., & Stroebe, M. (2010). Beyond depression: Yearning for the loss of a loved one. OMEGA–Journal of Death and Dying, 61(2), 85101. doi:10.2190/OM.61.2.a
  55. Stroebe, W., Stroebe, M., Abakoumkin, G., & Schut, H. (1996). The role of loneliness and social support in adjustment to loss: A test of attachment versus stress theory. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 70, 12411249.
  56. Tanimukai, H., Adachi, H., Hirai, K., Matsui, T., Shimizu, M., Miyashita, M., & Shima, Y. (2015). Association between depressive symptoms and changes in sleep condition in the grieving process. Supportive Care in Cancer, 23, 19251931. doi:10.1007/s00520-014-2548-x
  57. Taylor, S., & Rachman, S. J. (1991). Fear of sadness. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 5, 375381. doi:10.1016/0887-6185(91)90037-T
  58. Van Baarsen, B., Van Duijn, M., Smit, J., Snijders, T., & Knipscheer, K. (2001). Patterns of adjustment to partner loss in old age: The widowhood adaptation longitudinal study. OMEGA–Journal of Death and Dying, 44, 536. doi:10.2190/PDUX-BE94-M4EL-0PDK
  59. Wakefield, J., & Schmitz, M. (2013). Normal vs. disordered bereavement-related depression: Are the differences real or tautological? Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 127, 159168. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0447.2012.01898.x
  60. Winnicott, D. (1953). Transitional objects and transitional phenomena. International Journal of Psycho Analysis, 34, 8997.
  61. Winnicott, D. (1965). The maturational processes and the facilitating environment. London, UK: Hogarth.
  62. Worden, J. W. (1976). Personal death awareness. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
  63. Worden, J. W. (1996). Tasks and mediators of mourning: A guideline for the mental health practitioner. In Session: Psychotherapy in Practice, 2, 7380. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1520-6572(199624)2:4<73::AID-SESS7>3.0.CO;2-9
  64. Worden, J. W., & Silverman, P. R. (1993). Grief and depression in newly widowed parents with school-age children. OMEGA–Journal of Death and Dying, 27, 251260. doi:10.2190/XMHJ-F977-P8GV-4W07
  65. Zisook, S., Corruble, E., Duan, N., Iglewicz, A., Karam, E. G., Lanouette, N., … Young, I. T. (2012). The bereavement exclusion and DSM-V. Depression & Anxiety, 29, 425443. doi:10.1002/da.21927
  66. Zisook, S., & Kendler, K. S. (2007). Is bereavement-related depression different than non-bereavement-related depression? Psychological Medicine, 37, 779794. doi:10.1017/S0033291707009865
  67. Zisook, S., Paulus, M., Shuchter, S. R., & Judd, L. L. (1997). The many faces of depression following spousal bereavement. Journal of Affective Disorders, 45, 8594.
  68. Zisook, S., & Shuchter, S. R. (1993). Major depression associated with widowhood. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 1, 316326. doi:10.1097/00019442-199300140-00006
  69. Zisook, S., & Shuchter, S. R. (2001). Treatment of the depressions of bereavement. American Behavioral Scientist, 44, 782792. doi:10.1177/0002764201044005006